Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Experiment 2 (N=10) Purpose: Examine the ability of rare abrupt onsets (20% of trials) to capture attention away from a relevant cue. Design: Half of the.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Experiment 2 (N=10) Purpose: Examine the ability of rare abrupt onsets (20% of trials) to capture attention away from a relevant cue. Design: Half of the."— Presentation transcript:

1 Experiment 2 (N=10) Purpose: Examine the ability of rare abrupt onsets (20% of trials) to capture attention away from a relevant cue. Design: Half of the participants responded to the red letter in the target display and the other half responded to the green letter. The cue display contained a box colored the same as the target letter, which would normally capture attention. On 80% of the trials, this relevant cue appeared by itself, and on the other 20% of trials, it had to compete with a simultaneous abrupt onset (the relevant+onset cue). The onset appeared in the same hemifield as the relevant cue (but not the same location) for 10% of the trials, but in the other hemifield for the other 10% of the trials (see below for examples for the target color red). Results and Discussion Behavioral Data: N2pc Effects: The relevant cue by itself elicited a substantial N2pc effect (-0.398 µV), ts(9)≥- 2.19, ps≤.05. When competing with an abrupt onset, the relevant cue still elicited a strong N2pc effect (-0.386 µV; ts(9)≥-1.33, ps ≤.22); the difference was not significant, F<1.0. Conclusions Using behavioral and electrophysiological measures, we found no evidence that salient-but-irrelevant objects (color singletons and abrupt onsets) have the power to capture spatial attention, even when presented rarely (20% of trials). We found that capture was driven primarily by top-down control settings, not bottom-up stimulus salience. Introduction Can salient objects capture our attention? Some have argued that the salient stimuli, such as color singletons, do not have inherent power to capture our spatial attention unless they contain some properties match those we are currently looking for (contingent capture; e.g., Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992; Lien, Ruthruff, Goodin, & Remington, 2008). Others have argued that frequent present presentation of the salient objects diminished their power to capture attention (e.g., Horstmann & Ansorge, 2006). The present study examined capture by salient objects when presented only occasionally, using both behavioral measures (response time [RT] and proportion of error [PE]) and electrophysiological measures. The Present Study The goal of the present study was to determine whether salient-but-irrelevant objects can trigger the involuntary capture when presented rarely. We used a cuing paradigm, with non-informative cues (25% valid vs. 75% invalid). To assess capture, we measured cue validity effects (e.g., shorter RT for valid trials than invalid trials). We also assessed capture using an electrophysiological measure - the N2pc - believed to reflect the allocation of spatial attention. When attention is directed to the left or right visual field, brain potentials are found to be more negative in the contralateral hemisphere than in the ipsilateral hemisphere. The N2pc is the difference in event-related brain potentials (ERPs) between these hemispheres. It is usually strongest over posterior scalp, about 170-270 ms after stimulus onset. The N2pc effect provides an index of whether and when attention has been directed to the cue. N2pc Effect = ERP contralateral to cue– ERP Ipsilateral to cue Experiment 1 (N = 16) Purpose: Determine whether an irrelevant color singleton has the power to capture spatial attention, producing a cuing validity effect and an N2pc, when it appears rarely. Target Display: Each participant responded to either the red letter, the green letter, the blue letter, or the yellow letter (4 groups). The target displays contained two T’s and two L’s. For the red and green target groups, one letter was red, one was green, and two were white. For the blue and yellow target groups, one letter was blue, one was yellow, and two were white. The two colored letters always appeared on opposite sides of the display. Cue Display: 80% of the trials (1024 trials in total) contained no cue, whereas 20% of the trials (256 trials in total) contained an irrelevant color singleton. For the red and green target groups, the color singleton cue was a blue box (among yellow boxes) or a yellow box (among blue boxes). For the blue and yellow target groups, the color singleton cue was a red box (among green boxes) or a green box (among red boxes). Sample Event Sequence: The target was red in this example. EEG Data Analyses: EEG epochs were time-locked to singleton cue onset. The cue-elicited N2pc was assessed 170-270 ms after cue onset; the target- elicited N2pc was assessed 350-450 ms after cue onset. Sites: O1, O2, PO5, PO6, P5, and P6 Predictions: If a salient-but-irrelevant singleton can capture attention when presented rarely, the cue should produce cue validity effects and N2pc effects. Results and Discussion Behavioral Data: N2pc Effects (cue present trials): The irrelevant color singleton cue produced no N2pc effect during the period 170-270 ms after cue onset, ts(15)<1.0, suggesting that it did not capture attention, despite being presented rarely. Electrophysiological Evidence for the Failure of Salient Stimuli to Capture Attention, even when Presented Rarely Birken Noesen & Mei-Ching Lien Eric Ruthruff Oregon State University University of New Mexico References Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030-1044. Horstmann, G., & Ansorge, U. (2006). Attentional shifts to rare singletons. Visual Cognition, 14, 295-325. Lien, M.-C., Ruthruff, E., Goodin, Z., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: Converging evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 509-530. Mean RT was not significantly different between the no-cue condition and the valid and invalid cue conditions, Fs(1,12)≤1.78, ps≥.2072, indicating that the salient- but-irrelevant cue did not capture attention. OR Cue displays for Red/Green targets OR Cue displays for Blue/Yellow targets T T L L FixationCue Tone Feedback 1200 ms Target 100 ms1200-1400 ms50 ms100 ms50 msUntil Response100 ms TIME 527 ms533 ms Cue and target in the same hemifield Cue and target in different hemifields Pooled Cue-Elicited N2pc Effects Target-Elicited N2pc Effects * p <.0001 53 ms*68 ms* Valid Invalid 10% Same Hemifield 80% Relevant + Onset Cue 10% Different Hemifields Relevant Cue Only Relevant cues (in the target color) produced large cue validity effects, regardless of whether there was a simultaneous abrupt onset, ts(9)≥7.68, ps<.0001. Instead of reducing capture by the relevant stimulus, the onset may have actually increased capture, F(1,8)=3.50, p=.09. Cue and target in the same hemifield Cue and target in different hemifields Relevant Cue Relevant + Onset Cue Target-Elicited N2pc Effects Cue-Elicited N2pc Effects Acknowledgements This research was supported by funding from Oregon State University Undergraduate Research, Innovation, Scholarship, and Creativity to Birken Noesen.


Download ppt "Experiment 2 (N=10) Purpose: Examine the ability of rare abrupt onsets (20% of trials) to capture attention away from a relevant cue. Design: Half of the."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google