Download presentation
Published byJeremy Nichols Modified over 9 years ago
1
Introduction to Work and Organizational Psychology
Gerhard Ohrband 8th lecture Workload and Task Allocation
2
Course structure Part I Introduction 1 Managing diversity
2 History and context for Work and Organizational Psychology / Roles and methods Part II People at work 3 Job Analysis and Design 4 Personal Selection 5 Training
3
Course structure 6 Performance Appraisal: Assessing and Developing Performance and Potential 7 Job Stress and Health Part III Human Factors at Work 8 Workload and Task Allocation 9 Work Environments and Performance 10 The Design and Use of Work Technology 11 Safety at Work
4
Course structure Part IV Organizations at Work
Leadership and management Work motivation Teams: the challenges of cooperative work Organizational development (OD)
5
Part III – Human Factors at work 8 Workload and Task Allocation
Outline: 1. What is workload? 2. Why assess workload? 3. Requirements for effective workload assessment 4. Workload assessment techniques 5. The application of workload assessment techniques 6. Automation and task allocation
6
1. What is workload? performance problems associated with tasks high in explicit demand (i.e., the overload or cognitive strain scenario) exploring/modelling errors at lower levels of the task demand spectrum
7
Physical vs. mental workload
Focus on mental workload Examples: air traffic controllers, pilots, process control operators and medical staff Primary tasks: cognitive processes which require memory, attention, perception and communication skills rather than extensive physical demands Definition (Kramer, 1991): Mental workload can be conceptualized as the costs that human operators incur in performing tasks
8
Industries traditionally defined as high risk
Defence Road transportation Railways Aerospace Process control Power generation
9
Workload and error The complexity of the relationship between workload, task performance and task load can be illustrated with reference to the debate in which a number ofinvestigators have aimed to provide an answer to the question “How much workload is too much?” (e.g., de Waard, 1996; Meijman and O’Hanlon, 1984;Teigen, 1994). To answer this question, investigators have found it useful todivide the Yerkes-Dodson inverted U function into 6 task performance-related regions as shown in Figure 8.
10
Task performance and workload as a function of demand
11
Single resource models
12
Kahneman’s capacity of attention
13
Performance Resource Function (PRF)
14
Multiple resource models
the human information processing system is a multiple channel processor (e.g., it has multiple structures) each processor, or group of processors, has its own internal capacity. In MRT approaches, mental resources are often seen as analogous to fuel that is consumed by various activities, or as a tank of liquid to be divided among several competing tasks (Wickens, 1984). In stressful conditions, or multitasking situations, the amount of resource may become depleted and give rise to interference effects. One important feature of this view is the idea that the impact ofchanges in task demand on mental resources may not be purely quantitative, but may also be qualitative as well, (i.e., structural).
16
Sources of mental resource relative to information processing stage
17
Diagrammatic representation of Wickens Multiple Resource Theory
18
Composite models relatively new type of cognitive modelling activity that aims to use elements of the single channel hypothesis and resource theory to account for workload effects in human performance. These composite models are usually represented in the form of computer programs that are applied to tasks and used to predict various aspects of human performance. The ability of these models to reproduce workload effects has frequently been mentioned as a justification for their development – although the extent to which they have managed to do this has been a matter of some debate. Perhaps the most frequently mentioned composite model in the workload literature has been the Executive-Process Interactive Control (EPIC) model developed by Kieras and Meyer, 1997
20
2. Why assess workload? Solve practical problems in the workplace
Effects of workload on performance, well-being, health and safety Design complex task environments that do not place disproportionate demands on the human operator 1. Safety critical systems such as air traffic control and aircraft cockpit design 2. Aid evaluation of the effects of automation or the introduction of new technology and other changes in the nature of work on individual well-being and health 3. Assessment of individual operators (selection or training) Acute vs. chronic effects of workload
21
3. Requirements for effective workload assessment
Sensitivity Diagnosticity Intrusiveness Validity Reliability Acceptability Applicability Generality
22
4. Workload assessment techniques
Subjective/self-report measures: Cooper-Harper scale (Cooper and Harper, 1969) NASA task load index (TLX) (Hart and Staveland, 1988) Subjective workload assessment technique (SWAT) (Reid and Nygren, 1988) Instantaneous self-assessment technique (ISA) Situation awareness rating scale (SART) (Taylor, 1989) Situation awareness global assessment technique (SAGAT) (Endsley, 1995)
24
NASA Task Load Index (TLX)
25
NASA-TLX Scoring example
26
Subjective Workload Assessment Technique - SWAT
27
Time Based Task Loading Models
28
Performance measures Primary task measures Secondary task measures
Primary task: the task or system function whose workload is to be measured Secondary task: one task that is performed concurrently with a primary task to investigate the workload associated with the latter
29
Physiological Measures
cardiac function brain function respiration eyeblinks pupil dilation urine blood saliva (hormonal and immunological changes)
30
5. The application of workload assessment techniques Summary of the capability of different broad categories of workload assessment techniques to satisfy different requirements for use Intrusiveness Sensitivity Diagnosticity Applicability Acceptability Subjective Measures Post-task measures Generally not intrusive Good but may depend on length of task Generally difficult to use diagnostically Minimal equipment requirements Very good Instantaneous measures Potentially intrusive Good Provides only a global measure Some equipment required good Performance measures Primary task Not intrusive Reasonable but difficulties in interpreting variation Poor Depends on task complexity and variability Should be acceptable to operator Secondary task Potential for intrusion May require training and extra equipment Additional demands may be distracting
31
Physiological measures
Intrusiveness Sensitivity Diagnosticity Applicability Acceptability Physiological measures EEG measures Not usually a problem Good Varies according to specific measure but reasonably good Extensive equipment and analysis requirements Some potential problems ECG measures Not intrusive Not fully established Not generally found to be problematical
32
6. Automation and task allocation
advancements in computer technology Wickens (1992): three reasons for automation 1. Situations which may be hazardous or dangerous to humans or which humans cannot perform (diving operations, handling of toxic materials) 2. Tasks with high levels of workload for the human operator (autopilots) 3. Overcome human limitations, e.g. in memory or attention (radar advance warning systems)
33
Dangers of automation Thoughtless design which may simply require the operator to perform those functions or tasks that have been unable to be automated Difficulties: 1. Automation may induce feelings of loss of control and situation awareness when the human is operating “out of the loop” 2. Risk of deskilling the operator in highly automated systems 3. Automation does not always lead to improved performance and levels of operator workload
34
Unscheduled manual interventions (Hockey and Maule, 1995)
operators overriding automated systems in order to assume control of production or other processes at times when the system is scheduled to be under automatic control Why? 1. desire for control 2. motivation to improve the speed and quality of production 3. low trust in the automated system
35
Selection of the appropriate type and degree of automation
Goal: achieving desirable levels of safety and effectiveness Question: Which tasks should be allocated to the operator and which to be automated part of the system? Task allocation/function allocation Traditional approaches: Fitt’s list (1951, Kantowitz and Sorkin, 1987) Think about it: Where are machines and where are humans more effective?
36
Answer: Machines: performing mathematical and computational operations, integrating information and dealing with predictable events reliably Human operators: making decisions, inductive reasoning, more flexible, particularly when unexpected events occur and possess experience of previous events
37
Dynamic task allocation (DTA)
Def.: flexible allocation of tasks or functions between the operator and the system in human-machine systems; sometimes referred to as adaptive control some or all of the task elements have the potential to be carried out by either the operator or the system itself control of task allocation: explicit and implicit allocation explicit: operator control, implicit: computer control of task allocation
38
Advantages of DTA 1. Workload of operators will be maintained at a relatively constant level 2. Resources of the systems (both human and computer) will be used more fully 3. More acceptances by the operators than static automation 4. Enhancement of situation awareness and prevention of decay of manual control and problem-solving skills which may be required in breakdown or emergency situations 5. Enhancement of the operator’s ability to diagnose failures and errors made by the computer
39
Discussion Points What are the different workload assessment techniques? Explain the hazards of automation. Discuss the objectives of workload assessment.
40
Literature Hockey, G.R.J. and Maule, A.J. (1995). Unscheduled manual interventions in automated process control. Ergonomics, 38, Kramer, A.F. (1991). Physiological metrics of mental workload: a review of recent progress. In D.L. Damos (ed.), Multiple-Task Performance. London: Taylor and Francis
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.